Debunking Antidepressants
Antidepressants work because they are placebos.
It seems as if the media are finally coming around to questioning the over-medicalization of American psychiatry and mental health services in general.
I recently wrote a piece debunking Ritalin and related stimulants for treating Attention Deficit Disorder.
On Sunday, 60 Minutes on CBS discussed Irving Kirsch's work showing that anti-depressants are no better than placebos for all but the most severe cases of depression.
Kirsch had to use the Freedom of Information Act to force drug companies to reveal studies they had done, and which they had suppressed, showing that the drugs don't work.
A key part of the show dealt--without using the terms--with two statistical concepts that might have been confused in the minds of some viewers. These are significance level and effect size.
Significance level refers to the likelihood that an experimental result could be due to chance--that is, that a study using different people would have shown no effect. In general, if there is less than a 5% chance that the results are a coincidence, then the finding is called significant. (That is, even when a drug has no effect, an experiment might by chance occasionally seem to produce one--so results of all experiments, positive and negative, need to be considered.) If the probability is much lower--say one in a thousand--then it is called highly significant.
This is a very different use of the word significant from everyday speech, where it means important or substantial. In statistics, a highly significant finding may be of no importance at all. Suppose that an experiment shows that a pill improves mildly depressed people's mood by 1%. If there is only a one in a million chance that that result was a coincidence, then the finding is statistically highly significant. However, it is clinically meaningless, since neither the depressed individuals nor others close to them would be able to detect a 1% improvement.
So when a drug company says that studies of the effectiveness of their antidepressant--the ones they allowed to be published--obtained "highly significant results" that doesn't necessarily tell you what you want to know. The important information is effect size. Once you know that the results are statistically significant, you need to quantify the treatment impact. For example, how much lower on a scale of depression do people score, or how many weeks less does their depression last?
There are statistical ways of combining findings from different studies; and Kirsch was making two important points. The first is that when you combine the suppressed studies with the published ones, statistical significance disappears for most people. The second is that, even when statistically significant results are obtained, only for the severely depressed is the effect size large enough to justify taking antidepressants.
What does work for depression? The same thing that makes the placebo effect work--anything that believably encourages hope and gets individuals to expect that their life will improve.
Structured therapies (e.g., cognitive and behavioral therapies) focus on reversing the downward spiral of depression by catching up on work and other tasks left undone, by recognizing and changing self-defeating thoughts and actions, and by building on strengths, interests, and individual and social assets. Studies comparing antidepressants and focused talk therapies have also shown therapy to be more effective in preventing relapses--presumably because individuals learn ways of thinking and acting that make depression less likely in the first place, and also help them to cope better when they feel down.
In addition, physical activity and exercise--probably by combating lethargy and rumination, and by increasing the oxygen supply to the brain--have also been shown to be helpful.
(Full disclosure: my first book, Placebo Therapy [Jossey-Bass, 1973], argued that psychotherapy works mainly because of the placebo effect; and that therapy should aim at maximizing that effect.)
Image Source:
Four Placebos
http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Four_Placebos.jpg
Prescription placebos used in research and practice
http://www.nlm.nih.gov/hmd/emotions/self.html
NIH, objects donated by Elaine and Arthur Shapiro
Check out my most recent book, The Myth of Race, which debunks common misconceptions, as well as my other books at http://amazon.com/Jefferson-M.-Fish/e/B001H6NFUI
The Myth of Race is available on Amazon http://amzn.to/10ykaRU and Barnes & Noble http://bit.ly/XPbB6E
Friend/Like me on Facebook: http://www.facebook.com/JeffersonFishAuthor
Follow me on Twitter: www.twitter.com/@jeffersonfish
Visit my website: www.jeffersonfish.com
A key part of the show dealt--without using the terms--with two statistical concepts that might have been confused in the minds of some viewers. These are significance level and effect size.
Significance level refers to the likelihood that an experimental result could be due to chance--that is, that a study using different people would have shown no effect. In general, if there is less than a 5% chance that the results are a coincidence, then the finding is called significant. (That is, even when a drug has no effect, an experiment might by chance occasionally seem to produce one--so results of all experiments, positive and negative, need to be considered.) If the probability is much lower--say one in a thousand--then it is called highly significant.
This is a very different use of the word significant from everyday speech, where it means important or substantial. In statistics, a highly significant finding may be of no importance at all. Suppose that an experiment shows that a pill improves mildly depressed people's mood by 1%. If there is only a one in a million chance that that result was a coincidence, then the finding is statistically highly significant. However, it is clinically meaningless, since neither the depressed individuals nor others close to them would be able to detect a 1% improvement.
So when a drug company says that studies of the effectiveness of their antidepressant--the ones they allowed to be published--obtained "highly significant results" that doesn't necessarily tell you what you want to know. The important information is effect size. Once you know that the results are statistically significant, you need to quantify the treatment impact. For example, how much lower on a scale of depression do people score, or how many weeks less does their depression last?
There are statistical ways of combining findings from different studies; and Kirsch was making two important points. The first is that when you combine the suppressed studies with the published ones, statistical significance disappears for most people. The second is that, even when statistically significant results are obtained, only for the severely depressed is the effect size large enough to justify taking antidepressants.
What does work for depression? The same thing that makes the placebo effect work--anything that believably encourages hope and gets individuals to expect that their life will improve.
Structured therapies (e.g., cognitive and behavioral therapies) focus on reversing the downward spiral of depression by catching up on work and other tasks left undone, by recognizing and changing self-defeating thoughts and actions, and by building on strengths, interests, and individual and social assets. Studies comparing antidepressants and focused talk therapies have also shown therapy to be more effective in preventing relapses--presumably because individuals learn ways of thinking and acting that make depression less likely in the first place, and also help them to cope better when they feel down.
In addition, physical activity and exercise--probably by combating lethargy and rumination, and by increasing the oxygen supply to the brain--have also been shown to be helpful.
(Full disclosure: my first book, Placebo Therapy [Jossey-Bass, 1973], argued that psychotherapy works mainly because of the placebo effect; and that therapy should aim at maximizing that effect.)
Image Source:
Four Placebos
http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Four_Placebos.jpg
Prescription placebos used in research and practice
http://www.nlm.nih.gov/hmd/emotions/self.html
NIH, objects donated by Elaine and Arthur Shapiro
Check out my most recent book, The Myth of Race, which debunks common misconceptions, as well as my other books at http://amazon.com/Jefferson-M.-Fish/e/B001H6NFUI
The Myth of Race is available on Amazon http://amzn.to/10ykaRU and Barnes & Noble http://bit.ly/XPbB6E
Friend/Like me on Facebook: http://www.facebook.com/JeffersonFishAuthor
Follow me on Twitter: www.twitter.com/@jeffersonfish
Visit my website: www.jeffersonfish.com
Subscribe to Looking in the Cultural Mirror
Looking in the Cultural Mirror
Recent Posts
Most Popular
-
1
-
2
-
3
-
4
-
5The Rare Truth about "Tight" and "Loose" Women
by Michael Castleman, M.A.
Sex Mudras: Energy Movement Exercises for Sexual Vitality
Serge Villecroix
Sacred gestures of the hands and body to energize the sexual organs, increase libido, enhance pleasure, and improve overall health.
Read more
Anxiety Free
A comprehensive formula with herbs and nutrients clinically proven to increase feelings of well-being.
Read more
Setria®
Setria® glutathione is a pure, all-natural ingredient that helps promote detoxification and provides antioxidant protection.*
Read more
Current Issue
What Happy People Do Differently The key to satisfaction lies within risk
More From This Issue
Issue Archives
No comments:
Post a Comment